• Nyheter
    • Pluss-innhold
    • Arbeidsliv
    • Psykologi-folk
    • Nye bøker
    • Podkaster og videoer
      • Pia og psyken
      • Psykologlunsj
      • Psykologisk salong
      • Videoer
  • Ideer
    • Ytringer
    • Bokutdrag
    • Spalter
      • Forebygg depresjon med Arne Holte
      • Fra terapirommet med Kirsti Jareg
      • Hverdagspsykologi med Eirik Hørthe
      • Kjærleik & liv med Anne Marie Fosse Teigen
      • Kritisk tenkning med Torstein Låg
      • Menneskets natur med Leif Edward Ottesen Kennair
      • Gutta fra Psykologlunsj
      • Månedens klassiker
  • Aktiviteter
  • Stillinger
  • Bli abonnent
  • Kontakt oss
    • Vil du annonsere?
    • Send innlegg
    • Ansatte
  • Nyheter
    • Pluss-innhold
    • Arbeidsliv
    • Psykologi-folk
    • Nye bøker
    • Podkaster og videoer
      • Pia og psyken
      • Psykologlunsj
      • Psykologisk salong
      • Videoer
  • Ideer
    • Ytringer
    • Bokutdrag
    • Spalter
      • Forebygg depresjon med Arne Holte
      • Fra terapirommet med Kirsti Jareg
      • Hverdagspsykologi med Eirik Hørthe
      • Kjærleik & liv med Anne Marie Fosse Teigen
      • Kritisk tenkning med Torstein Låg
      • Menneskets natur med Leif Edward Ottesen Kennair
      • Gutta fra Psykologlunsj
      • Månedens klassiker
  • Aktiviteter
  • Stillinger
  • Bli abonnent
  • Kontakt oss
    • Vil du annonsere?
    • Send innlegg
    • Ansatte

Scandinavian Psychologist.

  • Volumes
    • 6 – 2019
    • 5 – 2018
    • 4 – 2017
    • 3 – 2016
    • 2 – 2015
    • 1 – 2014
  • Aims and scope
  • Author guidelines
  • Advisory board
  • Volumes
    • 6 – 2019
    • 5 – 2018
    • 4 – 2017
    • 3 – 2016
    • 2 – 2015
    • 1 – 2014
  • Aims and scope
  • Author guidelines
  • Advisory board

Self-reported premorbid health in 15 individuals who later developed schizophrenia compared with healthy controls: Prospective data from the Young-HUNT1 Survey (The HUNT Study)

Norwegian adolescents in the premorbid phase of schizophrenia reported poorer self-perceived mental and psychosocial health in several areas compared with healthy controls, write Arne Okkenhaug and colleagues.

PREMORBID PHASE: Adolescents in the premorbid phase of schizophrenia felt more nervous, less happy, and sad, and they described themselves as more worried. Additionally, they had fewer friends, and they reported feeling lonely more often, write Arne Okkenhaug and colleagues. Illustration: Aurora Nordnes.

Arne Okkenhaug, Torbjørn Tanem, Tor Åge Myklebust, Bjørn Gjervan & Asbjørn Johansen

Last updated: 16.11.19  Published: 28.11.18

Citation

Okkenhaug, A., Tanem, T., Myklebust, T. Å., Gjervan, B., & Johansen, A. (2018). Self-reported premorbid health in 15 individuals who later developed schizophrenia compared with healthy controls: Prospective data from the Young-HUNT1 Survey (The HUNT Study). Scandinavian Psychologist, 5, e8. https://doi.org/10.15714/scandpsychol.5.e8

Abstract

Self-reported premorbid health in 15 individuals who later developed schizophrenia compared with healthy controls: Prospective data from the Young-HUNT1 Survey (The HUNT Study)

Objective: The study investigated whether youths who later developed schizophrenia would report poorer mental and psychosocial health compared with matched healthy controls. Method: The study had a prospective case control design. Data were extracted from the Norwegian health survey, the Young-HUNT1 survey (collected between 1995–1997). Results: The case group reported poorer subjective well-being, more negative emotions, and more difficulties related to social function compared to healthy controls. Several of them smoked nicotine daily and they were less physically active. Conclusions: The results support other studies showing that premorbid schizophrenia is associated with negative emotions and social disturbances. The results also reveal that negative subjective well-being and problems with smoking and inactivity were present before onset of the disease.

Keywords: help-seeking behaviour, The HUNT Study, lifestyle risks, loneliness, negative emotions, premorbid, schizophrenia, social relations, subjective well-being.

Author(s)

Author affiliations: Arne Okkenhaug – Scientific Unit, Department of Psychiatry, Levanger Hospital, North Trøndelag Hospital Trust, Levanger, Norway; Torbjørn Tanem – Nidaros DPS (District Psychiatric Centre), Division of Mental Health Care, St. Olavs Hospital, Trondheim, Norway; Tor Åge Myklebust – Department of Research and Innovation, Møre and Romsdal Hospital Trust, Ålesund, Norway; Bjørn Gjervan & Asbjørn Johansen – Department of Psychiatry, Levanger Hospital, North Trøndelag Hospital Trust, Levanger, Norway. Contact information: Arne Okkenhaug, Scientific Unit, Department of Psychiatry, Levanger Hospital, North Troendelag Hospital Trust, Pb. 333, 7601 Levanger, Norway. Email: arne.okkenhaug@hnt.no. Received: December 27, 2017. Accepted: October 16, 2018. Published: November 30, 2018. Language: English. Competing interests: The authors report no conflict of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the contents and writing of this paper. Acknowledgement: The Nord-Trøndelag Health Study (The HUNT Study) is a collaboration among The HUNT Research Centre (Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Norwegian University of Science and Technology NTNU), the Nord-Trøndelag County Council, the Central Norway Regional Health Authority, and the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. The authors thank Hans M. Nordahl, Professor, PhD and Ulla Romild, Statistician, PhD for grateful support in the first phases of the project and Catriona Turner for language checking. The project has received financial support from Nord University, Norway. This is a peer-reviewed paper.

News article

(In Norwegian:) As a group, young people who later suffered from schizophrenia reported significantly poorer self-perceived health than the control group. Read the interview with Torbjørn Tanem and Arne Okkenhaug in Psykologisk.no.

Forfatterinfo

Arne Okkenhaug

Arne Okkenhaug works at the Scientific Unit, Department of Psychiatry, Levanger Hospital, North Trøndelag Hospital Trust, Norway.

Torbjørn Tanem

Torbjørn Tanem is a specialist in psychology at Nidaros DPS (District Psychiatric Centre), Division of Mental Health Care, St. Olavs Hospital, Norway.

Tor Åge Myklebust

Tor Åge Myklebust is a statistician at Department of Research and Innovation, Møre and Romsdal Hospital Trust, Norway.

Bjørn Gjervan

Bjørn Gjervan works at the Department of Psychiatry, Levanger Hospital, North Trøndelag Hospital Trust, Norway.

Asbjørn Johansen

Asbjørn Johansen works at the Department of Psychiatry, Levanger Hospital, North Trøndelag Hospital Trust, Norway.

Early identification of individuals at risk of developing schizophrenia is crucial for developing effective preventive interventions (Cornblatt et al., 2015; Fusar-Poli et al., 2013; Laurens & Cullen, 2016; Rutigliano et al., 2018). The detection and prevention of psychotic symptoms has been of interest in both research and treatment intervention development since the early 2000s (Fusar-Poli et al., 2013; Millan et al., 2016). Delaying, ameliorating, and preventing the onset of schizophrenia is especially important considering the potentially chronic and debilitating consequences associated with the disorder (Cornblatt et al., 2003; Fusar-Poli et al., 2013; Laurens & Cullen, 2016; Millan et al., 2016; Morgan et al., 2014). The identification and investigation of risk factors and groups at ultra-high risk of developing schizophrenia has been a major aim of researchers, and temporary symptoms of psychosis and loss of function along with genetic risk factors have been regarded as early markers for later such development (Fusar-Poli et al., 2012, 2013). However, recent research has shown that identifying subjects who later develop psychosis is difficult. Schizophrenia shares risk factors with other mental disorders (Scott, 2016), and individuals defined as “at risk” of developing schizophrenia will not necessarily develop the disorder (Scott, 2016).

As a result of the high risk perspective, interventions have often been provided in the prodromal phase of schizophrenia (Brew, Shannon, Storey, Boyd, & Mulholland, 2017; Fusar-Poli, Yung, McGorry, & van Os, 2014). It may be more effective to investigate predictors in earlier stages of life in order to prevent development of the disorder. Previous evidence suggests that this earlier identification should be done prior to the advent of significant disability characterizing the prodrome and without restriction to those accessing health services (Laurens & Cullen, 2016; Scott, 2016). A number of researchers maintain that psychotherapeutic, psychosocial, or mastering-oriented interventions should be directed towards children and adolescents who are potentially in premorbid phases of the disorder (Laurens & Cullen, 2016; Liu, Keshavan, Tronick, & Seidman, 2015; Scott, 2016; Seidman & Nordentoft, 2015; Sommer et al., 2016). Neurocognitive impairments (Bora et al., 2014; Seidman et al., 2016) and reduced social skills (Addington & Heinssen, 2012) are often already established when subclinical symptoms emerge (Sommer et al., 2016). Multiple preventive and treatment strategies have been proposed for children with proven genetic vulnerability and children with transient psychotic symptoms (Sommer et al., 2016).

Relatively robust evidence indicates that children who present social, emotional, and behavioural problems as well as psychosis-related symptoms in childhood or adolescence have an increased risk of developing schizophrenia spectrum disorders later in life (Cornblatt et al., 2015; Golembo-Smith et al., 2012; Rapado-Castro, McGorry, Yung, Calvo, & Nelson, 2015; Welham, Isohanni, Jones, & McGrath, 2009). Previous studies have identified the developmental antecedents of schizophrenia prior to the prodromal phase, such as greater exposure and responsiveness to stressors, impaired performance on measures of general intelligence, specific cognitive functions, brain structure and functional abnormalities, and neuromotor dysfunction (Dickson, Laurens, Cullen, & Hodgins, 2012; Rund, 2018; Welham et al., 2009).

A fairly large number of studies have focused on retrospective information from peers, teachers, and relatives, while few prospective studies have investigated the subjects’ own experiences (Seidman & Nordentoft, 2015). Results from longitudinal studies of general populations as well as familial high risk in cases of individuals at increased genetic risk of developing schizophrenia may not be generalizable. That is because most people with the disorder do not have affected family members (Laurens & Cullen, 2016).

It is important to expand the knowledge of early antecedents of schizophrenia in order to develop specific interventions aimed at identification and prevention of early psychotic symptoms (Scott, 2016). Prospective data on antecedents are not widely available because many studies have investigated retrospective data (Laurens et al., 2015). However, there is evidence that self-report measures are reliable in premorbid phases of schizophrenia (Brill et al., 2007). Prospective data from individuals who later developed psychotic illness and who have described their subjective experiences and premorbid characteristics in terms of health-related variables help preclude biased recollections of historical behaviour. They could also be a useful contribution to the field of early detection and prevention of psychotic illness (Lancefield, Raudino, & Downs, 2016).

The aim of the study
The aim of the study was to compare self-reported health and function in premorbid participants who later developed schizophrenia with healthy controls. We hypothesized that those in the premorbid phase of schizophrenia would report poorer self-perceived mental and psychosocial health compared to the controls.

Method

Design
The study had a prospective case-control design based on premorbid data from the Young-HUNT1 survey, which was part of the Nord-Trøndelag Health Study (Helseundersøkelsen i Nord-Trøndelag, HUNT). This longitudinal population study was conducted by the Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU).

The Young-HUNT1 survey
The Young-HUNT1 survey was conducted in Nord-Trøndelag County in the period spanning 1995 to 1997 (Holmen et al., 2014) (https://www.ntnu.edu/hunt). Nord-Trøndelag is in Central Norway and currently has a stable population size of around 130,000 inhabitants (Holmen et al., 2014). The county is considered to be representative of Norway as a whole in terms of geography, economy, industry, and sources of income, age distribution, morbidity, and mortality. The study presented in this article was based on the Young-HUNT1 survey, which is the adolescent part of The HUNT Study (age range 13–19 years). The cohort consisted of 8,984 persons, about 90% of the youth population in Nord-Trøndelag. The data collection was performed in schools by trained nurses, and the methods included self-report questionnaires, structured interviews, and clinical measurements. The questionnaires covered major public health issues, including somatic and mental health, quality of life, and health behaviours.

Procedure
Based on a list of persons registered with a diagnosis of schizophrenia (F.20.0–F.20.9) in the Patient Administrative System (PAS) at the Department of Psychiatry of Nord-Trøndelag Hospital Trust, we identified people born in Nord-Trøndelag between 1977 and 1983 and thus eligible for participation in Young-HUNT1. Two study members examined the medical records of the PAS cohort based on the following inclusion criteria (Figure 1): 1) residence during the Young-HUNT1 survey period; 2) plausible schizophrenia diagnosis based on patient records; and 3) the absence of prodromal and manifest psychotic symptoms during Young-HUNT1 data collection. Prodromal symptoms were considered present in cases where marked changes in behaviour or thought patterns were reported at the time of participation in Young-HUNT1. The included cases were then invited to participate in the study.

Two external experienced psychiatrists validated the included cohorts’ diagnoses and estimated onset of disease based on the participants’ medical records. Inclusion was dependent upon fulfilment of the diagnostic criteria according to the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision (ICD-10) criteria (World Health Organisation, 2016). Each case was grouped in one of the following categories: (a) certain diagnosis; (b) probable diagnosis; and (c) uncertain diagnosis. If there was at least one ‘uncertain diagnosis’ score, then the respondents’ data were excluded. The project members confirmed the respondents’ participation in the Young-HUNT1 survey based on the HUNT database. An application for this procedure was granted by the NTNU administrators of The HUNT Study (Nord-Trøndelag Health Study, 2015).

The informed consent procedure was undertaken by project members in collaboration with the individual’s therapist, primarily in the clinic or at his or her home. When individuals were prevented from using this approach, they were contacted by telephone. When necessary, required information was obtained from medical records held at clinics for children and adolescents and from former therapists.

Individuals who fulfilled the inclusion criteria and gave their consent to participate in the study were included in the study case group. The case group consisted of patients from the Psychiatric Department in the Nord-Trøndelag Hospital Trust. 

FIGURE 1: Procedure for inclusion of study participants.

Sample
The study dataset consisted of responses to the self-reported Young-HUNT1 questionnaires from a case group of 15 youths (Table 1), of whom 12 were boys and three were girls. Mean age at the time of Young-HUNT1 data collection was 16 years and one month, ranging from 13 years and 11 months to 18 years and eight months. The time of first reported prodromal symptoms varied from six months to eight years after Young-HUNT1. Later, four subgroups of schizophrenia were identified among the case group subjects.

From the HUNT sample (n = 8,984), we excluded respondents who had reported reduced mobility, reduced hearing, physical illness, severe mental illness, epilepsy, diabetes, migraine, or other somatic conditions lasting more than three months. From the remaining cohort, we identified a comparison group of healthy controls, matched 3:1 (n = 45) on gender and age.

TABLE 1: Characteristics of study sample.

Measures
To measure topics relevant to this study, we selected the questions in Young-HUNT1 regarding psychiatric conditions such as anxiety and depression, self-esteem, personality, and well-being. In addition, we included questions related to alcohol use, tobacco use, physical activity, school problems, social function, help-seeking behaviour, and leisure-time activity. (For the questionnaire see Young-HUNT1; ‘Junior high school (age 13–16)’ and ‘High school (age 16–19)’).

Some of the questions were single items, but most of the independent variables were composite scores constructed by the administrators of HUNT from a set of questions (examples: school function (14), friends (7), alcohol (5), and tobacco use (10) as well as help-seeking behaviour (6)). Other HUNT Study questions were based on structured, validated scales like The Hopkins Symptom Checklist version SCL-5 (Tambs & Moum, 1993). This checklist was used to assess symptoms of anxiety and depression. Four questions from Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale were used to assess self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965). ‘General well-being’ is a three-item quality-of-life scale validated in earlier HUNT studies (Storksen, Roysamb, Holmen, & Tambs, 2006). Moreover, an 18-item short version of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) assesses extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1977). Psychoticism has been difficult to interpret in theory (Gudbergsson, Fosså, Sanne, & Dahl, 2007) and was therefore excluded from our analyses. Each item on the EPQ-18 was scored as 0 (no) and 1 (yes), and the six-item scores on each dimension were summed, giving a total score ranging from zero to six on each dimension. We present results on all the sum scores but also for each question.

This dual presentation was done since some of the single questions, such as “often worried,” might be particularly important considering the subjects’ risk of potentially experiencing emotional problems prior to schizophrenia development (Rapado-Castro et al., 2015; Welham et al., 2009).

The question ‘Do you smoke?’ had five items ranging from ‘Yes, I smoke about xx cigarettes daily’ to ‘No, I don’t smoke’. The responses were dichotomized between ‘smoking daily’ and ‘not smoking daily’. Similar dichotomizations have been used in other Young-HUNT studies (Bratberg, Nilsen, Holmen, & Vatten, 2007; Mangerud, Bjerkeset, Holmen, Lydersen, & Indredavik, 2014). Schizophrenia is associated with an increased risk of daily smoking (Compton et al., 2009). Activities during the past week were dichotomized into ‘inactivity’ and ‘activity’. Inactivity was defined as ‘less than one day or one hour or less per week’ in accordance with recommendations in other studies (Rangul, Holmen, Kurtze, Cuypers, & Midthjell, 2008). Self-reported alcohol use was assessed with two questions: (1) ‘Have you ever tried drinking alcohol?’ with the response options ‘Yes’, ‘No’, and ‘Don’t know’; and (2) ‘Have you ever drunk so much alcohol that you felt intoxicated (drunk)?’, which was measured on a scale with five response options (‘Never’, ‘Once’, ‘2–3 times’, ‘4–10 times’, and ‘> 10 times’). None of the participants answered ‘Don’t know’ to the first question, which was dichotomized into ‘Yes’ or ‘No’. The second question was dichotomized between ‘Never’ and ‘Once or more’. Similar dichotomizations have been used in other Young-HUNT studies (Bratberg et al., 2007; Mangerud et al., 2014).

Lacking existing dichotomization procedures in the Young-HUNT1 dataset, we divided the groups based on their absence or presence of the element asked for in the questions. The question ‘How often have you been to the school health centre?’, which had three values – ‘Not at all’, ‘One to three times’, and More than three times’– the variables were dichotomized between ‘Not at all’ and ‘One or more times’.

Leisure time was assessed with the question: ‘How often have you done any of these activities in the past week?’ Since problems related to social activities are found to be a potentially important risk factor for schizophrenia, we present only those five of the nine sub-questions related to social activities (Derdikman-Eiron, Hjemdal, Lydersen, Bratberg, & Indredavik, 2013; Liu et al., 2015; Tarbox & Pogue-Geile, 2008; Michalska da Rocha, Rhodes, Vasilopoulou, & Hutton, 2017).

Fourteen sub-questions about school were gathered under the main question: ‘Do any of the following things happen to you at school/concerning school, or have any of them happened?’ The school questions were grouped into three separate domains: (1) problems related to concentration and academic achievement (Academic); (2) behavioural problems (Conduct); and (3) well-being problems (Dissatisfaction) (Storksen, Roysamb, Holmen, & Tambs, 2006). A specific single question, ‘Are you teased/harassed by other students?’, formed a single question domain. All of these are presented in the results section.

Adding to the already referred items, we used three single-item questions that investigated participants’ experience of loneliness, number of friends, and self-assessed health status.

Statistical methods
Data analyses were performed with IBM’s SPSS, version 23 (IBM, 2013). Differences in means were analysed with independent samples’ t-tests. Associations between case and controls and the categorical variables were analysed by estimating odds ratios from standard binary logistic regressions and by chi-square or Fisher’s exact tests. All tests were two-tailed.

Ethical considerations
The study was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics and was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration (The World Medical Association, 2013). All participants had the required competence to give an informed consent to participate in the study.

Results

Participants in the case group reported significantly poorer scores on the ‘General well-being’ scale compared with those reported by the healthy controls (p = .002). The estimated mean difference was −2.5. On two of three items, the case group reported significantly poorer well-being compared with the healthy controls (satisfaction with life (p = .007) and feeling strong and vital (p = .001)).

TABLE 2: Mean differences in scale variables.

The scores on the SCL-5 scale showed no significant differences between the two groups. However, the case group scored significantly lower on one of the items, having been more dejected, down, or sad compared with the healthy controls (p = .01), and the mean difference was –.59. The participants in the case group experienced more nervousness (during the last month) than the controls (p = .004). With regard to the EPQ, there were no differences on the two scales for neuroticism and extraversion, but there were differences on two single items: ‘more often worried’ (p = .004) and not liking ‘meeting ahead of schedule to appointments’ (p = .013). We did not find any significant differences between the groups in terms of self-esteem as measured with the short version of Rosenberg`s Self-Esteem Scale.

Compared with healthy controls, those in the case group reported having fewer close friends (p = .034), feeling lonelier (p = .003), and spending fewer hours (p = .002) and days per week (p =.009) engaged in sports or exercise.

TABLE 3: Associations between case/controls and categorical variables.

TABLE 4: Continuous variables.

The case group reported daily smoking more often than the healthy controls (p = .001), but there were no differences between the groups on the question ‘Have you tried smoking?’

No differences were identified regarding the use of school health services or help-seeking behaviour based on the question about initiating contact with school health services. Those in the case group had more often been to a general practitioner (p = .011) and to a psychologist in the last year (p = .046) than had the healthy controls. There were no differences between the cases and controls in terms of patterns of alcohol use or the question about their present state of health (p = .251).

No differences were identified concerning school (concentration/school conduct and academic achievement).

Discussion

The study hypothesis was that, as a group, young people who later suffered from schizophrenia would report significantly poorer self-perceived health than would the control group. We found this was the case for some health-related themes. In many areas, there were no differences. None of the results indicated that the case group had better health than the healthy controls.

The findings suggest that the main differences between the case group and the control group were that the case group’s subjects had elevated premorbid levels of negative affect in terms of poorer subjective well-being and a higher degree of nervousness. They reported themselves more dejected and sad compared with the healthy controls. These findings correspond closely with results from previous research on emotional problems, which showed that, for example, social anxiety (Johnstone, Ebmeier, Miller, Owens, & Lawrie, 2005; Jones, Rodgers, Murray, & Marmot, 1994) and depressive symptoms (Fusar-Poli et al., 2017; Fusar-Poli, Nelson, Valmaggia, Yung, & McGuire, 2014; Häfner et al., 1998) were generally highly prevalent in early stages of non-affective psychotic disorders (Thompson et al., 2015).

To our knowledge, few studies have examined subjective well-being in people in phases prior to the onset of schizophrenia. When asked about both intrapsychic and functional conditions, young people who were considered vulnerable to developing psychosis reported lower subjective quality of life than control groups (Bechdolf & Pukrop et al., 2005; Bechdolf & Ruhrmann et al., 2005; Svirskis et al., 2007). Low level of well-being is associated with less resilience in people with mental illnesses (Uzenoff et al., 2010), and it is an important topic to investigate in people in transition to psychosis (Brew et al., 2017). For many of our participants, their experiences of poor levels of well-being appear to have existed for quite some time before the onset of schizophrenia. Therefore, the low well-being scores can hardly be explained by annoying psychotic symptoms or by negative mental and social consequences associated with having a diagnosis.

The participants reported being more affected by worrying compared to the healthy controls. The concept of worry is documented as a factor in theoretical models for the establishment and continuation of psychopathological processes (Wells & Matthews, 1996). A thinking style characterized by worry has been identified as one among other specific proximal causal factors in persecutory delusions (Freeman & Garety, 2014). Worry leads to implausible ideas (Freeman et al., 2012; Freeman & Garety, 2014). Antecedent worry and rumination may predict delusional and hallucinatory experiences and associated distress (Hartley, Haddock, e Sa, Emsley, & Barrowclough, 2014). An elevated level of worry may contribute to a longer duration of paranoid thoughts (Startup, Freeman, & Garety, 2007). Treatment aimed at reducing concern may improve paranoid symptoms (Freeman et al., 2015). Our findings indicate that worrying processes were present in premorbid phases for the case group subjects. This finding may serve as a contribution to the ongoing discussion of the phenomenon’s significance in the development of schizophrenia.

The case group reported poorer social functioning compared with healthy controls regarding the following: feeling lonelier, having fewer close friends, not following up on appointments, and having a tendency to receive fewer visits. Studies have indicated that the frequency of meeting friends and the experience of low subjective well-being are the strongest adolescent predictors of reduced psychosocial functioning in young adulthood (Derdikman-Eiron et al., 2013). A number of studies have shown that a history of poor childhood social functioning during critical developmental stages is a sensitive predictor for schizophrenia later in life (Liu et al., 2015; Tarbox & Pogue-Geile, 2008). Loneliness is not only associated with manifest psychotic disorders but is also likely to be already present in subclinical stages (Michalska da Rocha et al., 2017). For a number of our cases, the experience of loneliness was reported before the onset of schizophrenia, indicating that such an experience was potentially a contributing factor in the development of the disorder.

Based on the responses they gave, the participants of the Young-HUNT1 survey were dissatisfied with their lives, often did not feel strong and vital, did not like school breaks, and felt lonelier than their peers. From this information, we can assume that many of them who later developed schizophrenia experienced struggles in daily life. A connection between low subjective well-being, depression, and experienced social support has previously been identified in individuals with first-episode psychosis (Uzenoff et al., 2010). Furthermore, previous research suggests that greater sensitivity to everyday stress is an indicator of both later development of psychosis (Myin-Germeys & van Os, 2007; Van Winkel, Stefanis, & Myin-Germeys, 2008) and experiences of social defeat (Selten & Cantor-Graae, 2007). The aforementioned dissatisfaction with life could indicate that at the time of participation the case subjects experienced what Myin-Germeys & Van Os (2007) describe as the ‘emotional pathway to psychosis’.

There were no differences between the case and control groups on the variables ‘self-esteem’ and ‘school function’ (concentration and academic achievement). The homogeneous response on self-esteem is interesting, given that the case group reported a higher degree of emotional and social problems. The lack of significant differences on reported school function differs from studies that have shown an association between schizophrenia in the premorbid phase and IQ reduction, cognitive deficits (Agnew-Blais et al., 2015; Welham et al., 2009), lower grades at school, and more negative academic assessments from teachers (Ullman, Levine, Reichenberg, & Rabinowitz, 2012).

Our study showed that individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia were less physically active in the premorbid phase of their disease compared with the healthy controls. In an earlier study, we found a relationship between inactivity and a diagnosis of schizophrenia (Okkenhaug et al., 2016). The reduced physical activity in the premorbid phases of schizophrenia development in this study confirm the findings from three linked studies of physical activity in adolescents who later developed psychosis (Sormunen et al., 2017).

The majority of the case group subjects reported that they smoked daily. There is a strong association between nicotine use and schizophrenia (Gurillo, Jauhar, Murray, & MacCabe, 2015; McCreadie, 2003), especially with daily smoking (Compton et al., 2009). Earlier studies have documented that smoking is overrepresented among people in the pre-clinical phases of schizophrenia (Myles et al., 2012) and at the onset of first-episode psychoses (Gurillo et al., 2015). Smoking is also associated with the earlier debut of symptoms (Gurillo et al., 2015). That many in the case group reported smoking before onset of disease supports the current argument that smoking is not necessarily a cause of schizophrenia. Smoking could represent one of many risk factors for disease development (Gurillo et al., 2015) by contributing to symptoms such as anxiety and depression (Alderson & Lawrie, 2015).

Studies have shown that alcohol abuse starts in early adolescence and that there are no differences in such abuse between healthy youths and youths in premorbid phases of schizophrenia (Buchy et al., 2015; Hambrecht, Lotz, Häfner-Ranabauer, & Waschkowski, 1996; Mangerud et al., 2014). Our findings support the findings of these studies.

Some case group subjects reported that they had more often attended appointments with general practitioners or psychologists. However, we found no difference between the groups as to whether they had taken the initiative to seek help from their school health services. Moreover, none of the participants reported the need for additional help of that type. Previous research has shown that people with first-episode psychosis do not initiate help-seeking for themselves, particularly those who have a relative who is affected by mental illness (O’Callaghan et al., 2010). This lack of help-seeking increases the challenge of implementing early identification and intervention strategies. Identification of adolescent premorbid function should rely on information not only from health services but also from peers, teachers, or significant others.

Limitations and strengths
The clinical sample in the present study was small. This tendency may have increased the risk of reduced external validity and generalizability. The sample size also excluded the possibility of conducting multiple regression analysis and justified the choice to analyse dichotomous variables. Furthermore, the sample had considerable gender skewness, with more males than females, which might have affected the results. There was also a risk of reduced reliability associated with the reporting of self-reported data, but there is no reason to suspect that participants with a later diagnosis of schizophrenia would differ from others regarding the validity of such data (Brill et al., 2007). The risk of bias regarding data derived from self-assessments stemming from participants’ long-term deficient memories or subjective distortions of their childhood was minimized by the data being prospective.

The case group participants had well-validated diagnoses, and people with short, single psychotic episodes (< 1 month) were excluded. The case group subjects were also considered representative of the total population of people who developed schizophrenia in the cohort born between 1977 and 1983 in Nord-Trøndelag. Compared with other studies where respondents were selected on the basis of genetic risk factors, the self-reported premorbid data limited potentially biased responses compared with other retrospective methods.

Conclusions

Adolescents in the premorbid phase of schizophrenia reported poorer self-perceived mental and psychosocial health in several areas compared with healthy controls. Compared with the healthy controls, they reported poorer subjective well-being, they felt more nervous, less happy, and sad, and they described themselves as more worried. Additionally, they had fewer friends, and they reported feeling lonely more often. Almost 50% smoked nicotine daily, and they were less physically active than their healthy peers.

The task of identifying subjects at risk of developing schizophrenia is challenging.

Despite the limited possibility to generalize the findings, our results may support earlier empirical studies that examined possible strategies for the early detection and prevention of disease development. Implementing strategies aimed at adolescents of multiple age levels presupposes a broad knowledge base regarding markers for premorbid development. Our findings suggest that such markers could consist not only of manifest symptoms but also, to a greater extent, vague characteristics and problems.

The task of identifying subjects at risk of developing schizophrenia is challenging because such individuals may not seek help for their psychological problems. It can be assumed that data from broad national health surveys, such as Young-HUNT1, will continue to be an important source of knowledge. The variety of questions on multiple domains provides the opportunity to construct a comprehensive knowledge base for the heterogeneous premorbid characteristics of people at risk of developing schizophrenia. More prospective data from larger cohorts are needed to be able to generalize such knowledge.

References

Addington, J., & Heinssen, R. (2012). Prediction and prevention of psychosis in youth at clinical high risk. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8(1), 269–289. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143146

Agnew-Blais, J. C., Buka, S. L., Fitzmaurice, G. M., Smoller, J. W., Goldstein, J. M., & Seidman, L. J. (2015). Early childhood IQ trajectories in individuals later developing schizophrenia and affective psychoses in the New England family studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(4), 817–823. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbv027

Alderson, H. L., & Lawrie, S. M. (2015). Does cigarette smoking cause psychosis? The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(8), 672–673. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00239-4

Bechdolf, A., Pukrop, R., Köhn, D., Tschinkel, S., Veith, V., Schultze-Lutter, F., … Klosterkötter, J. (2005). Subjective quality of life in subjects at risk for a first episode of psychosis: A comparison with first episode schizophrenia patients and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Research, 79(1), 137–143. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.008

Bechdolf, A., Ruhrmann, S., Wagner, M., Kühn, K. U., Janssen, B., Bottlender, R., … Klosterkötter, J. (2005). Interventions in the initial prodromal states of psychosis in Germany: Concept and recruitment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187(SUPPL. 48), 45–48. doi:10.1192/bjp.187.48.s45

Bora, E., Lin, A., Wood, S. J., Yung, A. R., Mcgorry, P. D., & Pantelis, C. (2014). Cognitive deficits in youth with familial and clinical high risk to psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 130(1), 1–15. doi:10.1111/acps.12261

Brew, B., Shannon, C., Storey, L., Boyd, A., & Mulholland, C. (2017). A qualitative phenomenological analysis of the subjective experience and understanding of the at risk mental state. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 12(1). doi:10.1080/17482631.2017.1342504

Bratberg, G. H., Nilsen, T. I. L., Holmen, T. L., & Vatten, L. J. (2007). Perceived pubertal timing, pubertal status and the prevalence of alcohol drinking and cigarette smoking in early and late adolescence: A population based study of 8950 Norwegian boys and girls. Acta Paediatrica, 96(2), 292–295. doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00102.x

Brill, N., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., Harary, E., Lubin, G., Davidson, M., & Weiser, M. (2007). Accuracy of self-reported premorbid functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 97(1–3), 103–108. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.026

Buchy, L., Cadenhead, K. S., Cannon, T. D., Cornblatt, B. A., McGlashan, T. H., Perkins, D. O., … Addington, J. (2015). Substance use in individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 45(11), 2275–2284. doi:10.1017/S0033291715000227

Compton, M. T., Kelley, M. E., Ramsay, C. E., Pringle, M., Goulding, S. M., Esterberg, M. L., … Walker, E. F. (2009). Association of pre-onset cannabis, alcohol, and tobacco use with age at onset of prodome and age at onset of psychosis in first-episode patients. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(11), 1251–1257. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09030311

Cornblatt, B. A., Carrión, R. E., Auther, A., McLaughlin, D., Olsen, R. H., John, M., & Correll, C. U. (2015). Psychosis prevention: A modified clinical high risk perspective from the recognition and prevention (RAP) Program. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(10), 986–994. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.13121686

Cornblatt, B. A., Lencz, T., Smith, C. W., Correu, C. U., Auther, A. M., & Nakayama, E. (2003). The schizophrenia prodrome revisited: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(4), 633–651. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007036

Derdikman-Eiron, R., Hjemdal, O., Lydersen, S., Bratberg, G. H., & Indredavik, M. S. (2013). Adolescent predictors and associates of psychosocial functioning in young men and women: 11-year follow-up findings from the Nord-Troendelag Health Study. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 54(2), 95–101. doi:10.1111/sjop.12036

Dickson, H., Laurens, K. R., Cullen, A. E., & Hodgins, S. (2012). Meta-analyses of cognitive and motor function in youth aged 16 years and younger who subsequently develop schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 42(4), 743–755. doi:10.1017/S0033291711001693

Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1977). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire: EPQ-R Adult. San Diego, CA: Educational and industrial testing service.

Freeman, D., Dunn, G., Startup, H., Pugh, K., Cordwell, J., Mander, H., … Kingdon, D. (2015). Effects of cognitive behaviour therapy for worry on persecutory delusions in patients with psychosis (WIT): A parallel, single-blind, randomised controlled trial with a mediation analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(4), 305–313. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00039-5

Freeman, D., & Garety, P. (2014). Advances in understanding and treating persecutory delusions: A review. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(8), 1179–1189. doi:10.1007/s00127-014-0928-7

Freeman, D., Stahl, D., McManus, S., Meltzer, H., Brugha, T., Wiles, N., & Bebbington, P. (2012). Insomnia, worry, anxiety and depression as predictors of the occurrence and persistence of paranoid thinking. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47(8), 1195–1203. doi:10.1007/s00127-011-0433-1

Fusar-Poli, P., Bonoldi, I., Yung, A. R., Borgwardt, S., Kempton, M. J., Valmaggia, L., … McGuire, P. (2012). Predicting psychosis: Meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(3), 220–229. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1472

Fusar-Poli, P., Borgwardt, S., Bechdolf, A., Addington, J., Riecher-Rössler, A., Schultze-Lutter, F., … Yung, A. (2013). The psychosis high-risk state: A comprehensive state-of-the-art review. Archives of General Psychiatry, 70(1). doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269

Fusar-Poli, P., Nelson, B., Valmaggia, L., Yung, A. R., & McGuire, P. K. (2014). Comorbid depressive and anxiety disorders in 509 individuals with an at-risk mental state: Impact on psychopathology and transition to psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(1), 120–131. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs136

Fusar-Poli, P., Tantardini, M., De Simone, S., Ramella-Cravaro, V., Oliver, D., Kingdon, J., … Mcguire, P. (2017). Deconstructing vulnerability for psychosis: Meta-analysis of environmental risk factors for psychosis in subjects at ultra high-risk. European Psychiatry, 40, 65–75. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.09.003

Fusar-Poli, P., Yung, A. R., McGorry, P., & van Os, J. (2014). Lessons learned from the psychosis high-risk state: Towards a general staging model of prodromal intervention. Psychological Medicine, 44, 17–24. doi:10.1017/S0033291713000184

Golembo-Smith, S., Schiffman, J., Kline, E., Sørensen, H. J., Mortensen, E. L., Stapleton, L., … Mednick, S. (2012). Premorbid multivariate markers of neurodevelopmental instability in the prediction of adult schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: A high-risk prospective investigation. Schizophrenia Research, 139, 129–135. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.05.012

Gudbergsson, S. B., Fosså, S. D., Sanne, B., & Dahl, A. A. (2007). A controlled study of job strain in primary-treated cancer patients without metastases. Acta Oncologica, 46(4), 534–544. doi:10.1080/02841860601156132

Gurillo, P., Jauhar, S., Murray, R. M., & MacCabe, J. H. (2015). Does tobacco use cause psychosis? Systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(8), 718–725. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00152-2

Hambrecht, M., Lotz, M., Häfner-Ranabauer, W., & Waschkowski, H. (1996). Sozialarbeit in der station{ä}ren psychiatrischen VersorgungEine empirische Studie zu Indikation und Erfolg. Der Nervenarzt, 67(11), 953–959. doi:10.1007/s001150050077

Hartley, S., Haddock, G., e Sa, D. V., Emsley, R., & Barrowclough, C. (2014). An experience sampling study of worry and rumination in psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 44(8), 1605–1614. doi:10.1017/S0033291713002080

Holmen, T. L., Bratberg, G., Krokstad, S., Langhammer, A., Hveem, K., Midthjell, K., … Holmen, J. (2014). Cohort profile of the Young-HUNT study, Norway: A population-based study of adolescents. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43, 536–544. doi:10.1093/ije/dys232

Häfner, H., Maurer, K., Löffler, W., an der Heiden, W., Munk-Jørgensen, P., Hambrecht, M., & Riecher-Rössler, A. (1998). The ABC schizophrenia study: A preliminary overview of the results. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33(8), 380–386. doi:10.1007/s001270050069

IBM (2013). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Computer Program, Amonk, New York: IBM Corporation.

Johnstone, E. C., Ebmeier, K. P., Miller, P., Owens, D. G. C., & Lawrie, S. M. (2005). Predicting schizophrenia: Findings from the Edinburgh high-risk study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186(JAN), 18–25. doi:10.1192/bjp.186.1.18

Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., & Marmot, M. (1994). Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. The Lancet (London, England), 344(8934), 1398–1402. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(94)90569-X

Lancefield, K. S., Raudino, A., & Downs, J. M. (2016). Trajectories of childhood internalizing and externalizing psychopathology and psychotic-like experiences in adolescence : A prospective population-based cohort study. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 527–536. doi:10.1017/S0954579415001108

Laurens, K. R., & Cullen, A. E. (2016). Toward earlier identification and preventative intervention in schizophrenia: Evidence from the London Child Health and Development Study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. doi:10.1007/s00127-015-1151-x

Laurens, K. R., Luo, L., Matheson, S. L., Carr, V. J., Raudino, A., Harris, F., & Green, M. J. (2015). Common or distinct pathways to psychosis? A systematic review of evidence from prospective studies for developmental risk factors and antecedents of the schizophrenia spectrum disorders and affective psychoses. BMC Psychiatry, 15, 205. doi:10.1186/s12888-015-0562-2

Liu, C. H., Keshavan, M. S., Tronick, E., & Seidman, L. J. (2015). Perinatal Risks and Childhood Premorbid Indicators of Later Psychosis: Next Steps for Early Psychosocial Interventions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(4), 801–816. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbv047

Mangerud, W. L., Bjerkeset, O., Holmen, T. L., Lydersen, S., & Indredavik, M. S. (2014). Smoking, alcohol consumption, and drug use among adolescents with psychiatric disorders compared with a population based sample. Journal of Adolescence, 37(7), 1189–1199. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.08.007

McCreadie, R. G. (2003). Diet, smoking and cardiovascular risk in people with schizophrenia: Descriptive study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 183(6), 534–539. doi:10.1192/bjp.183.6.534

Michalska da Rocha, B., Rhodes, S., Vasilopoulou, E., & Hutton, P. (2017). Loneliness in Psychosis: A Meta-analytical Review. Schizophrenia Bulletin. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx036

Millan, M. J., Andrieux, A., Bartzokis, G., Cadenhead, K., Dazzan, P., Fusar-Poli, P., … Weinberger, D. (2016). Altering the course of schizophrenia: Progress and perspectives. Nature Reviews. Drug Discovery, 15(7), 485–515. doi:10.1038/nrd.2016.28

Morgan, C., Lappin, J., Heslin, M., Donoghue, K., Lomas, B., Reininghaus, U., … Dazzan, P. (2014). Reappraising the long-term course and outcome of psychotic disorders: The AESOP-10 study. Psychological Medicine, 44(13), 2713–2726. doi:10.1017/S0033291714000282

Myin-Germeys, I., & van Os, J. (2007). Stress-reactivity in psychosis: Evidence for an affective pathway to psychosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 27(4), 409–424. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.005

Myles, N., Newall, H. D., Curtis, J., Nielssen, O., Shiers, D., & Large, M. (2012). Tobacco use before, at, and after first-episode psychosis: A systematic meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 73(4), 468–475. doi:10.4088/JCP.11r07222

North-Trondelag Health Study (2015). Guidelines for administration and use of research data from The Nord-Trøndelag Health Study (HUNT). Trondheim: NTNU. https://www.ntnu.edu/documents/140075/0/Guidelines_for_the_use_of_HUNT_data.pdf/1a597987-8149-4a5f-a427-d2435ade310e

O’Callaghan, E., Turner, N., Renwick, L., Jackson, D., Sutton, M., Foley, S. D., … Kinsella, A. (2010). First episode psychosis and the trail to secondary care: Help-seeking and health-system delays. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45, 381–391. doi:10.1007/s00127-009-0081-x

Okkenhaug, A., Tanem, T., Johansen, A., Romild, U. K., Nordahl, H. M., & Gjervan, B. (2016). Physical activity in adolescents who later developed schizophrenia: A prospective case-control study from the Young-HUNT. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 70(2). doi:10.3109/08039488.2015.1055300

Rangul, V., Holmen, T. L., Kurtze, N., Cuypers, K., & Midthjell, K. (2008). Reliability and validity of two frequently used self-administered physical activity questionnaires in adolescents. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 8, 1–10. doi:10.1186/1471-2288-8-47

Rapado-Castro, M., McGorry, P. D., Yung, A., Calvo, A., & Nelson, B. (2015). Sources of clinical distress in young people at ultra high risk of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, (165), 15–21. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.022

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Science, 148(3671), 804. doi:10.1126/science.148.3671.804

Rund, B. R. (2018). The research evidence for schizophrenia as a neurodevelopmental disorder. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 59(1), 49–58. doi:10.1111/sjop.12414

Rutigliano, G., Merlino, S., Minichino, A., Patel, R., Davies, C., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2018). Long term outcomes of acute and transient psychotic disorders : The missed opportunity of preventive interventions. European Psychiatry, 52(August), 126–133. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.05.004

Scott, J. G. (2016). Childhood antecedents of schizophrenia: Will understanding aetiopathogenesis result in schizophrenia prevention? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51(4), 493–495. doi:10.1007/s00127-016-1184-9

Seidman, L. J., & Nordentoft, M. (2015). New targets for prevention of schizophrenia: Is it time for interventions in the premorbid phase? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(4), 795–800. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbv050

Seidman, L. J., Shapiro, D. I., Stone, W. S., Woodberry, K. A., Ronzio, A., Cornblatt, B. A., … Woods, S. W. (2016). Association of neurocognition with transition to psychosis: Baseline functioning in the second phase of the North American prodrome longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(12), 1239–1248. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2479

Selten, J. P., & Cantor-Graae, E. (2007). Hypothesis: Social defeat is a risk factor for schizophrenia? British Journal of Psychiatry. doi:10.1192/bjp.191.51.s9

Sommer, I. E., Bearden, C. E., van Dellen, E., Breetvelt, E. J., Duijff, S. N., Maijer, K., … Vorstman, J. A. (2016). Early interventions in risk groups for schizophrenia: What are we waiting for? NPJ Schizophrenia, 2(1). doi:10.1038/npjschz.2016.3

Sormunen, E., Saarinen, M. M., Salokangas, R. K. R., Telama, R., Hutri-Kähönen, N., Tammelin, T., … Hietala, J. (2017). Effects of childhood and adolescence physical activity patterns on psychosis risk—a general population cohort study. NPJ Schizophrenia, 3, 5. doi:10.1038/s41537-016-0007-z

Startup, H., Freeman, D., & Garety, P. A. (2007). Persecutory delusions and catastrophic worry in psychosis: Developing the understanding of delusion distress and persistence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(3), 523–537. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.04.006

Storksen, I., Roysamb, E., Holmen, T. L., & Tambs, K. (2006). Adolescent adjustment and well-being: Effects of parental divorce and distress. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47(1), 75–84. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00494.x

Svirskis, T., Korkeila, J., Heinimaa, M., Huttunen, J., Ilonen, T., Ristkari, T., … Salokangas, R. K. R. (2007). Quality of life and functioning ability in subjects vulnerable to psychosis. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 48(2), 155–160. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.10.008

Tambs, K., & Moum, T. (1993). How well can a few questionnaire items indicate anxiety and depression? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 87(5), 364–367.

Tarbox, S. I., & Pogue-Geile, M. F. (2008). Development of social functioning in preschizophrenia children and adolescents: A systematic review. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 561–583. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.34.4.561

The World Medical Association (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA, 310(20), 2191–2194. doi:10.1001/jama.2013.281053

Thompson, E., Kline, E., Ellman, L. M., Mittal, V., Reeves, G. M., & Schiffman, J. (2015). Emotional and behavioral symptomatology reported by help-seeking youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 162(1–3), 79–85. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.023

Ullman, V. Z., Levine, S. Z., Reichenberg, A., & Rabinowitz, J. (2012). Real-world premorbid functioning in schizophrenia and affective disorders during the early teenage years: A population-based study of school grades and teacher ratings. Schizophrenia Research, 136(1–3), 13–18. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.021

Uzenoff, S. R., Brewer, K. C., Perkins, D. O., Johnson, D. P., Mueser, K. T., & Penn, D. L. (2010). Psychological well-being among individuals with first-episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 4(2), 174–181. doi:10.1111/j.1751-7893.2010.00178.x

Van Winkel, R., Stefanis, N. C., & Myin-Germeys, I. (2008). Psychosocial stress and psychosis. A review of the neurobiological mechanisms and the evidence for gene-stress interaction. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(6), 1095–1105. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn101

Welham, J., Isohanni, M., Jones, P., & McGrath, J. (2009). The antecedents of schizophrenia: A review of birth cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(3), 603–623. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn084

Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1996). Modelling cognition in emotional disorder: The S-REF model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(11–12), 881–888. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00050-2

World Health Organisation (2016). ICD-10. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (10th Revis). Geneva: World Health Organisation.

Redaksjonen anbefaler

Det finnes veier ut av håpløsheten

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Unngående tilknytning: Når partneren avviser følelsene dine – og sine egne

  • Nyheter, Pluss

To gutter som mediterer – pusten førte dem sammen

  • Nye bøker, Nyheter, Pluss

Sykelig narsissisme: – Jeg tenker at det er en selvfølelse på speed

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Bipolar type 1 og 2: Ulike lidelser, men lignende løsninger

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Nyutdannet psykolog: – Det kom til et punkt hvor jeg druknet i pasienter

  • Nyheter, Pluss

God kommunikasjon redder ekteskap som lider av «phubbing»

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Highasakite-Ingrid: – Jeg har vært god på å lage noe fint ut av noe vondt

  • Nyheter, Pluss

ME-forsker mistenker at sykdommen skyldes immunsvikt

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Hva sier mødre er grunnen til at de mistet kontakt med sine voksne barn?

  • Nyheter, Pluss, Ukas forskning

Maren ville ikke dø alene. Men telefonen hun ringte til, reddet i stedet livet hennes

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Ut av depresjon: – Slik snur du den destruktive sirkelen

  • Nye bøker, Nyheter, Pluss

I møtet med selvmord valgte Rebekka åpenhet

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Mangler du glede, motivasjon og livslyst? Da lider du kanskje av anhedoni

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Anne B. Ragde drar heller på hytta enn til psykolog

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Tilknytning: Når barndommen gjentar seg i parforholdet

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Hva funker for å øke trivsel og mestring på jobb? Ikke stressmestringskurs, ifølge denne studien

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Omfattende studie avdekker hvordan traumer i barndommen endrer hjernens utvikling

  • Nyheter, Pluss

ME-syke Merethe følte seg ikke forstått. Det fikk fatale konsekvenser

  • Nyheter, Pluss

– Like mye som emosjonelt ustabile personer misforstår andre, misforstår andre dem

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Slik snakker du med ungdom om et annerledes utseende

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Hvorfor utvikler noen unnvikende personlighets­forstyrrelse?

  • Nye bøker, Nyheter, Pluss

Gaslighting – en psykologisk teknikk for å destabilisere noens forstand og virkelighets­forståelse

  • Hverdagspsykologi med Eirik Hørthe, Pluss

Åtte psykologi-filmer du kan nyte i regnværet

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Dette er de vanligste barndoms­traumene

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Slik kan følelser bli til hodepine og magesmerter

  • Nye bøker, Nyheter, Pluss

Fastlegen mener vi bør ignorere flere helseråd og bli mer fornøyde med det vi allerede gjør

  • Nye bøker, Nyheter, Pluss

Finnes det positive sider ved angst?

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Sanna Sarromaa var fanget i et psykisk voldelig forhold: – Det kan skje den sterkeste

  • Nye bøker, Nyheter, Pluss

Traumer eller ikke traumer – hvor går grensa?

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Siste saker

Å få innsikt i livshistorier bidrar til økt forståelse i Eidskog kommune

  • Nyheter, Pluss

– Du vil jo få det bedre, men du vil også være syk nok til å få hjelp

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Om dette er i tråd med norske lover og regler, har vi et problem

  • Ytringer

Menn topper selvmords­statistikken: – På en måte dobbelt utsatt ved traumer

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Hvordan gamle mønstre kan få oss til å bli i usunne forhold

  • Ytringer

En kvinnelobby mobiliserer og ødelegger saklig debatt om gode boordninger for barna

  • Ytringer

Chatbot-terapi viser lovende resultater: – Ser ingen grunn til at vi som profesjon skal føle oss truet

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Åtte rusbehandlinger saksøkte Helse Sør-Øst – vant i retten

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Skam er den mest smertefulle følelsen vi har

  • Nyheter, Pluss

– Vi deler pasientenes verste øyeblikk. Det er veldig verdifullt

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Makten til å definere andre som kronisk psykisk syke, er livsfarlig

  • Ytringer

Derfor var Per Isdal ekspertvitne i Ingebrigtsen-saken: – Retten bør se at vold er mer enn fysisk vold

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Oppropet til Stine Sofies Stiftelse er et tragisk bomskudd

  • Ytringer

De jobbet med barnevern i Russland: – Det var ganske brutalt på innsiden

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Depresjon ødelegger motivasjonen – også etter at depresjonen er over

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Du må ikke ofre noe for å nå målene dine – du må prioritere

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Casual sex skaper narsissisme

  • Ytringer

Han var nær ved å gi opp å forske på barns opplevelser på barnehus, men studien hans fikk følger

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Skjult depresjon: – Mange skjønner ikke at de er deprimerte

  • Nyheter, Pluss

ME er noe helt annet enn langvarig utmattelse

  • Ytringer

Burde mennesker med traumer få medisiner?

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Gaza har blitt psykologens blindflekk

  • Ytringer

Den smertefulle lojalitetskonflikten og usynlige kampen i Ingebrigtsen-saken

  • Ytringer

Veien ut av depresjon går gjennom andres medfølelse

  • Nyheter, Pluss

Mest lest

– Den vanligste personlighets­forstyrrelsen er lettest å overse

    Visse livsstiler øker faren for demens betraktelig

      – Psykisk vold dreper kjærlighet

        Gaslighting: – En ondskapsfull teknikk for å ta kontroll over et annet menneske

          Slik utnytter narsissisten din emosjonelle intelligens

            Oppdaget mulig årsak til emosjonelt ustabil personlighets­forstyrrelse

              Sinte voksne barn

                Nevroforsker om ADHD-diagnosen: – Det er ikke en enhetlig tilstand

                  Dette skjer med oss når vi opplever det mystiske fenomenet dissosiasjon

                    En bestemt oppførsel hos barn kan være tegn på senere angstlidelse

                      Hva skal til for å komme over et traume?

                        Dette er den skjulte formen for narsissisme

                          Slik er kjærlighetslivet med en narsissist

                            De tre søylene for god psykisk helse

                              Pia la om kostholdet og ble kvitt angsten

                                Tegnene på at du sliter med kronisk stress

                                  Med én enkel påstand kan du nå avsløre om noen lyver

                                    Tre faktorer kan svært presist forutsi psykiske lidelser

                                      Nye følelsesfunn i dypet av høysensitive hjerner

                                        Narsissisme – kan du holde ut?

                                          Hvorfor er det så vanskelig å gjøre det slutt?

                                            Noen personlighetstrekk beskytter mot demens – andre øker faren

                                              Emosjonelt intelligente foreldre blir oftere utbrent, antyder ny studie. Det kan skade foreldreevnen deres, mener forskerne

                                                Er du et A- eller B-menneske? Det henger sammen med personligheten din

                                                  Meld deg på nyhetsbrev fra Psykologisk.no

                                                  • Psykologisk.no AS​
                                                    C. J. Hambros plass 5
                                                    0164 Oslo
                                                    912 389 782 MVA
                                                  • Tips oss
                                                  • Kundeservice
                                                  • Skriv innlegg
                                                  • Bli annonsør
                                                  • Redaksjon
                                                  • Personvern
                                                  • Ansvarlig redaktør
                                                    Pål Johan Karlsen
                                                  • Nyhetsredaktør
                                                    Jonas Hartford Sundquist
                                                  • Administrasjons­sjef
                                                    Vera Thorvarsdottir
                                                  Facebook-f Twitter Linkedin

                                                  Psykologisk.no er medlem av Mediebedriftenes Landsforening og Fagpressen, og arbeider etter Vær Varsom-plakatens regler for god presseskikk.

                                                  Kopibeskyttet © 2025